iconOpen Access

ARTICLE

crossmark

Scarcity and Mental Health—Multiple Mediators of Sleep Quality and Life Satisfaction

by Na Liu1, Yan Zhang2, Junxiu Wang3,4,*

1 School of Sociology and Ethnology, University of Chinese Academy of Social Sciences, Beijing, 102488, China
2 Institute of Sociology, Chinese Academy of Social Science, Beijing, 100732, China
3 School of Mental Health, Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, 325000, China
4 Institute of Psychosocial Services, Kangning Hospital of Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, 325000, China

* Corresponding Author: Junxiu Wang. Email: email

(This article belongs to the Special Issue: Social Stress, Adversity, and Mental Health in Transitional China)

International Journal of Mental Health Promotion 2024, 26(6), 449-462. https://doi.org/10.32604/ijmhp.2024.049334

Abstract

Background: In the current social environment, scarcity, as a universally present objective state, profoundly impacts individuals’ decision-making and health through the subjective feeling it induces, known as a “scarcity mindset.” Particularly, the feeling of scarcity related to money and sleep time is not only widespread but also directly linked to an individual’s mental health. Purpose: This study aims to delve into the relationship between the feeling of scarcity and mental health, with a specific focus on the relationship between the feeling of money scarcity or sleep time scarcity and mental health, as well as the role of sleep quality or life satisfaction in this relationship. Procedure: We determined the sampling quotas based on the population and economic development levels of each province in the “China Statistical Yearbook (2021)” (National Bureau of Statistics, 2021). Participants were selected using the Probability Proportional to Size (PPS) sampling method. Data was collected by distributing online questionnaires to participants, and the relationships between the main variables were explored using structural equation modeling. Results: 1. In China, the feeling of sleep time scarcity is stronger than the feeling of money scarcity among the public. 2. The feeling of money scarcity is positively correlated with depression and anxiety, whereas the feeling of sleep time scarcity is only positively correlated with depression. 3. The feeling of scarcity mainly leads to depression and self-denial through reducing life satisfaction, and it leads to anxiety through reducing sleep quality. Conclusion: The feeling of scarcity in money and sleep time is related to different dimensions of mental health. Therefore, reasonably planning financial allocation and ensuring an adequate amount of sleep can reduce the sense of scarcity, thereby improving mental health. Additionally, improving sleep quality and increasing life satisfaction can alleviate the adverse effects of scarcity on mental health.

Keywords


Introduction

Currently, China is facing financial risk issues, with interconnections between real estate, investment, land acquisition, and government debt, resulting in a combination of three risk factors that have a certain impact on short-term economic development. Additionally, many large companies have initiated a wave of “layoffs,” leading to a significant impact on the mental and physical health of a large number of young people. Furthermore, China’s economic transformation has led to a redefinition of social classes, with some individuals quickly becoming wealthy while others fall into poverty. These economic instabilities and inequalities deepen people’s anxiety and the feeling of scarcity regarding money, prompting many individuals to choose to alleviate money anxiety by extending their working hours. According to data from the “China Labor Statistical Yearbook,” the average weekly working hours of urban employed individuals in China have steadily increased from 45.5-h in 2015 to 47.6-h [1], exceeding the legal daily 8-hour workday and the 44-h workweek system [2]. The extension of working hours directly results in a reduction of individuals’ non-working time, giving rise to the feeling of scarcity of time. During non-working hours, sleep time accounts for a significant proportion and competes with leisure time. Since individuals cannot create additional time, they often borrow time from their sleep [3], leading to the feeling of scarcity of sleep time. Therefore, scarcity has become a societal issue that is prevalent in everyday life [4,5]. Among the various feelings of scarcity, the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of scarcity of time are the most common, and this lack can be both actual and perceived [6]. However, objectively, the lack of money or time does not equate to the feeling of scarcity; the feeling of scarcity is a “mindset” that reflects people’s existential anxiety. This study aims to explore the application of the feeling of scarcity in the field of health, focusing specifically on the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of scarcity of sleep time, as well as the mediating effects of sleep quality and life satisfaction on mental health. By analyzing the relationships between these factors, we aim to reveal the potential impact pathways of the feeling of scarcity on individual mental health, providing a deeper understanding and guidance for promoting healthy lifestyles and improving mental health.

Hypothesis Development

Scarcity of money and scarcity of sleep time

According to the scarcity theory, individuals experience the feeling of scarcity when they perceive a need for more resources than they currently possess [7,8]. The theory of limited resources posits that human attention and cognitive resources are finite [9]. During problem-solving, cognitive activities consume an individual’s cognitive resources. If the total cognitive resources required by these activities exceed the individual’s available resources, the feeling of scarcity arises. The feeling of scarcity is a “mindset” that influences what people focus on and the trade-offs they make, affecting cognitive processes and the neural mechanisms related to decision-making, and ultimately influencing people’s decisions and behaviors [8,10,11]. For individuals, the most prevalent feeling of resource scarcity stems from money and time, and the feeling of money scarcity is the most prevalent and fundamental feeling among low-income groups [8,12]. The feeling of money scarcity is the subjective feeling that individuals feel inadequate about the money resources they have, and it is not only related to income but also to the individual’s money management, consumption habits, debt burden, and many other factors. Therefore, based on the viewpoint of scarcity theory, we define “money scarcity” as a feeling that individuals believe that the money resources, they have can hardly satisfy their economic needs. Time is also an important social and health resource [13], and sufficient disposable time can enhance life satisfaction and happiness [14]. In addition to objective time scarcity, we argue that subjective perceptions of time scarcity can likewise have an impact on an individual’s behavior and health. This study further focuses on time scarcity in sleep behavior, defining individuals’ subjective perception of whether their sleep time is sufficient as “the feeling of sleep time scarcity.” It reflects a feeling where individuals believe that the “sleep time they have” cannot meet their “sleep needs.” Short sleep duration may not be related to sleep quality, whereas the feeling of sleep scarcity may directly affect the quality of sleep.

Money scarcity and sleep time scarcity

Sleep is an important process of recovery and repair for the body and brain. A good night’s sleep can help an individual regain strength and energy, promote the normal functioning of body systems. Research has found that sleep is closely related to cognitive function throughout the entire lifespan [15]. Lifestyle factors, including sleep, are associated with cognitive function and the risk of dementia [16], with approximately 15% of attention deficit disorders possibly being caused by sleep problems [17]. Therefore, improving sleep quality is beneficial for enhancing cognitive function. A study by Irwin et al. demonstrated an interaction between sleep and the immune system, with sleep disorders contributing to inflammatory disorders and major depressive disorder, and inflammatory processes affecting sleep [18]. Sleep can affect various immune functions, and good sleep can reduce the risk of infection in the human body [19]. Thus, good sleep quality contributes to the maintenance of normal functioning of the immune system and thus improves health. Chronic sleep deprivation (e.g., short sleep duration, sleep disorders) can lead to inflammation [20], increasing the prevalence of cardiovascular disease [2125], diabetes mellitus, and obesity, among others [26,27]. In addition, sleep deprivation can lead to distraction, slow response, and reduced cognitive ability [25,28], and can also lead to increased psychological stress, induce psychological problems such as anxiety and depression [29]. This shows that sleep quality affects not only the physical health of individuals but also their psychological health.

It has been found that time scarcity can affect health by limiting the amount of time available to engage in healthy lifestyles [3033]. It has also been associated with stress, illness, security, and sleep quality [13,34,35]. Adequate sleep duration does not exactly mean good sleep quality. Because sleep duration is an objective indicator, whereas sleep quality is a subjective indicator, and subjective sleep duration perception is more closely related to sleep quality than objective sleep duration, we hypothesize that subjective perception of sleep duration together with sleep quality will jointly affect the individual’s heath status. In addition, the vast majority of current studies have only revealed the negative impact of objective money scarcity (e.g., low-income or low socioeconomic status) on sleep quality [36,37] but have rarely explored the mechanisms by which perceptions of money scarcity affect sleep quality. Therefore, we propose the hypothesis:

H1. Sleep quality plays a mediating role in the relationship between the feeling of scarcity and mental health.

H1a: The feeling of money scarcity reduces sleep quality, leading to depression.

H1b: The feeling of money scarcity reduces sleep quality, leading to anxiety.

H1c: The feeling of money scarcity reduces sleep quality, leading to self-denial.

H1d: The feeling of sleep time scarcity reduces sleep quality, leading to depression.

H1e: The feeling of sleep time scarcity reduces sleep quality, leading to anxiety.

H1f: The feeling of sleep time scarcity reduces sleep quality, leading to self-denial.

Life satisfaction and mental health

The concept of life satisfaction was first proposed by psychologist Ed Diener in 1984, who defined life satisfaction as an individual’s overall evaluation of and satisfaction with his or her life [38]. Diener believes that money can increase life satisfaction to a certain extent, but it is not the sole influencing factor [39], factors such as individual personality traits [40], economic status [41], social support [42], and health status [43] can have a significant impact on life satisfaction. In addition, an individual’s health status is closely related to life satisfaction. Individuals with poor health conditions are more likely to experience poor work environments compared to healthy people, and negative work conditions are associated with lower life satisfaction [44]. Older adults with good somatic functioning had higher life satisfaction; older adults with better mental health were more satisfied with their lives compared to those with average mental health [45]. At the same time, life satisfaction contributes to an individual’s mental health and quality of life [46,47]. In addition, social environments where people’s life satisfaction is higher are more likely to achieve stability and harmony and have higher levels of economic development and innovation [48,49].

This shows that life satisfaction is not only related to self-assessed health [5052] but also closely linked to psychological health [5357]. Also, specific domains of life satisfaction are associated with specific physical health outcomes, health behaviors, and social factors [58,59]. Studies based on longitudinal data from birth cohorts have demonstrated robust interactions between life satisfaction and mental health [60]. In addition, life satisfaction has been shown to act as a mediating variable in influencing an individual’s health behavior choices [61,62] and emotional state [6366].

Studies have shown that objectively low income or low socioeconomic class can lead to reduced life satisfaction or health status [6771]. However, we believe that subjective feelings also play an important role. For example, individuals who feel that resources are scarce or that they are in a low socioeconomic class may develop feelings of self-depreciation and doubt about their abilities and sense of worth. This negative self-evaluation may lead to mental health problems, such as anxiety and depression [72], which can reduce well-being [68]. In addition, they may compare themselves to those who have more resources or are in a higher socioeconomic class, further influencing their mental health and sense of happiness [73]. In addition, time scarcity has also been shown to reduce well-being [74] and lead to anxiety and insomnia [34,35]. However, few studies have addressed the mechanism of the effect of sleep time scarcity on mental health, and therefore, we propose the hypothesis:

H2. Life satisfaction plays a mediating role in the relationship between the feeling of scarcity and mental health.

H2a: The feeling of money scarcity reduces life satisfaction, leading to depression.

H2b: The feeling of money scarcity reduces life satisfaction, leading to anxiety.

H2c: The feeling of money scarcity reduces life satisfaction, leading to self-denial.

H2d: The feeling of sleep time scarcity reduces life satisfaction, leading to depression.

H2e: The feeling of sleep time scarcity reduces life satisfaction, leading to anxiety.

H2f: The feeling of sleep time scarcity reduces life satisfaction, leading to self-denial.

In summary, the theoretical model of this study is shown in Fig. 1.

images

Figure 1: Theoretical model.

Materials and Methods

Data sources

The data used in this study came from an online survey on “Sleep Status of Chinese Residents” conducted in November 2021 by the Institute of Sociology, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences (CASS). The Probability Proportional to Size (PPS) sampling method was used to accurately determine the sampling quota for each province, taking into account the population size and economic development of each province as provided in the China Statistical Yearbook (2021) (National Bureau of Statistics, 2021). The survey samples were all between the ages of 18 and 71 and were drawn from 27 provinces, autonomous regions, and municipalities directly under the central government, except Tibet, Ningxia, Qinghai, Hainan, Hong Kong, Macao, and Taiwan. After excluding invalid questionnaires such as omitted answers, wrong answers, and random answers, the final sample was 4194 valid samples. The average age of the samples was 29.16 ± 9.33 years old, of which 2421 (57.73%) were male and 1773 (42.27%) were female. Although the exclusion of invalid questionnaires resulted in a slight imbalance in the male-female ratio in the sample data, in order to maintain and respect the authenticity of the measurement results, we retained all eligible samples after excluding invalid questionnaires. For detailed information on the samples, please refer to Table 1.

images

Variables

Mental health

The dependent variable in this study was mental health, which was measured using the General Health Questionnaire GHQ-20 revised by Li et al. [75]. The questionnaire consists of 20 questions that are categorized into three dimensions: depression, anxiety, and self-affirmation. Each question has two options, “Yes” and “No”, where 0 = “No” and 1 = “Yes”. Scores were calculated by summing the scores of the items. The depression dimension consists of 6 questions, so the depression score ranges from 0 to 6, with higher scores indicating higher levels of depression. The anxiety dimension consists of 5 questions, so the anxiety score ranges from 0 to 5, with higher scores indicating higher levels of anxiety. The self-affirmation dimension consists of 9 questions, so the self-affirmation score ranges from 0 to 9. For ease of interpretation, the self-affirmation dimension was reverse coded to the self-denial dimension, where higher scores indicate higher levels of self-denial. Therefore, the higher the total scores for depression, anxiety, and self-denial dimensions, the more severe the psychological issues. It has been validated that the questionnaire and its depression, anxiety, and self-denial dimensions have Cronbach’s α coefficients of 0.823, 0.781, 0.857, and 0.809, respectively, indicating good internal consistency. In this study, the scale demonstrated good model fit (χ2 = 1453.987, df = 148, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.964, TLI = 0.954, RMSEA = 0.046, SRMR = 0.046). In this study, depression, anxiety, and self-denial are considered continuous variables.

The feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity

The independent variables in this study were the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity. In particular, the feeling of money scarcity was measured with the question “Do you have enough money?” 1 question item, where 1 = “completely enough”, 2 = “mostly enough”, 3 = “enough (generally)”, 4 = “rarely enough”, 5 = “not enough at all”, the score is between 1 and 5, the higher the score, the stronger the feeling of money scarcity; the feeling of sleep time scarcity is measured by the question “In the past month, do you think you slept long enough?” 1 question item, where 1 = “too much”, 2 = “a little too much”, 3 = “just right”, 4 = “not quite enough”, and 5 = “not enough at all”, with scores ranging from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating a greater feeling of sleep time scarcity. Both were considered as continuous variables in this study.

Sleep quality

A mediating variable in this study was sleep quality, which was measured using the Chinese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index scale. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) was proposed by Buysse et al. in 1989 [76], translated into Chinese by Liu Xianchen et al. in 1996, and initially validated for its measurement reliability and validity. Later, because of its simplicity and ease of use, as well as its reliability and validity in different populations, which have been adequately tested, it has become one of the most frequently used scales in sleep quality research studies. The PSQI is suitable for the investigation of the sleep status of the general population in the last month. The self-assessment part of the scale includes seven factors: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep persistence, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disorders, use of sleep medication, and daytime dysfunction, each factor score ranges from 0 to 3, with higher scores indicating poorer sleep quality. The total score of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index ranges from 0 to 21, with higher scores indicating poorer sleep quality. It has been validated that the questionnaire has a Cronbach’s α coefficient of 0.726, indicating excellent internal consistency. In this study, the scale demonstrated good model fit (χ2 = 125.82, df = 8, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.981, TLI = 0.949, RMSEA = 0.059, SRMR = 0.023). In this study, sleep quality was considered as a continuous variable.

Life satisfaction

In this study, another mediating variable is life satisfaction, using the life satisfaction scale proposed by Diener [77], which consists of 5 items, expressed as “My life is roughly in line with my ideals.” “My life situation is very satisfactory.” “I am satisfied with my life.” “Until now, I have been able to get the important things I would like to have in life.” “Even if life could be started over, there is nothing I would want to change.” These 5 question items were measured where 1 = “strongly disagree”, 2 = “disagree”, 3 = “not agree”, 4 = “neutral “, 5 = “agree more”, 6 = “agree”, and 7 = “strongly agree”, and final life satisfaction score was taken as the average of the five questions, with scores ranging from 1 to 7. average score, which ranges from 1 to 7, with higher scores indicating higher life satisfaction. The Cronbach’s α coefficient of the questionnaire was verified to be 0.912, which has good internal consistency. In this study, the scale demonstrated good model fit (c2 = 196.059, df = 5, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.987, TLI = 0.975, RMSEA = 0.095, SRMR = 0.019). In this study, life satisfaction is considered as a continuous variable.

Data analysis

Utilize SPSS 26 software to conduct descriptive statistical analysis and correlation analysis on the feeling of money scarcity, the feeling of sleep time scarcity, life satisfaction, sleep quality, depression, anxiety, and self-denial among individuals. Employ Amos 28 software to conduct path analysis and mediating effect testing on the relationships between these seven main variables. The highest level of significance for this study was p < 0.05.

Results

Descriptive statistical analysis and correlation analysis of the main variables

As seen in Table 2, the mean value of people’s sense of money scarcity is 3.021, with scores between “general” (=3) and “seldom enough” (=4), which indicates that people generally feel that their money is not quite enough, i.e., there is a slight sense of money scarcity. The mean value of people’s sense of scarcity of sleep time is 3.621, with scores between “just enough” (=3) and “not quite enough” (=4), which indicates that people generally feel that their sleep time is not quite enough, i.e., a stronger sense of scarcity of sleep time. The average PSQI score for the public is 6.373, which is below the median value, which indicates that people generally perceive their sleep quality to be good; and the mean value of people’s life satisfaction is 4.324 (SD = 0.684), with scores ranging between “neutral” (=4) and “agree more” (=5), indicating that people’s life satisfaction is slightly above average; the scores for depression, anxiety, and self-denial among the public are all below the median for these dimensions. This suggests that people’s levels of depression, anxiety, and self-denial are relatively low, indicating a good mental health status.

images

In addition, the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity are significantly positively correlated with depression, anxiety, and self-denial, indicating that the stronger the feeling of money scarcity or sleep time scarcity, the higher the levels of depression, anxiety, and self-denial. Specifically, the strongest positive correlation is between the feeling of money scarcity and depression, followed by anxiety and self-denial; while the feeling of sleep time scarcity shows the strongest positive correlation with anxiety, followed by depression and self-denial. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index is significantly positively correlated with depression, anxiety, and self-denial, indicating that the poorer the sleep quality, the higher the levels of depression, anxiety, and self-denial. Specifically, the strongest positive correlation is between PSQI and anxiety, followed by depression and self-denial. Life satisfaction is significantly negatively correlated with depression, anxiety, and self-denial, indicating that the lower the life satisfaction, the higher the levels of depression, anxiety, and self-denial. Specifically, the strongest negative correlation is between life satisfaction and self-denial, followed by depression and anxiety. The feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity are both significantly negatively correlated with life satisfaction, meaning that the weaker the feeling of money scarcity or sleep time scarcity, the higher the life satisfaction. Specifically, the feeling of money scarcity shows a stronger negative correlation with life satisfaction. PSQI is significantly negatively correlated with life satisfaction, indicating that the better the sleep quality, the higher the life satisfaction. The feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity are both significantly positively correlated with PSQI, meaning that the weaker the feeling of money scarcity or sleep time scarcity, the better the sleep quality. Specifically, the feeling of sleep time scarcity shows a stronger positive correlation with PSQI. The feeling of money scarcity is significantly positively correlated with the feeling of sleep time scarcity, indicating that the stronger the feeling of money scarcity, the stronger the feeling of sleep time scarcity.

Path analysis

As shown in Table 3, we have tested the fit of our theoretical model in this study. Due to the large sample size, the CMID/DF is slightly higher but still less than 10. Additionally, considering that other indicator values are relatively high, it can be concluded that the fit of the model is good.

images

The results of the standardized path analysis shown in Table 4 indicate that, except for the non-significant direct effects of the feeling of money scarcity on self-denial, the feeling of sleep time scarcity on anxiety, and self-denial, the direct effects of the other paths are all significant. The model can explain 31.2% of the variance in life satisfaction, with the direct effect of the feeling of money scarcity on life satisfaction (β = −0.5, p < 0.001) being greater than the direct effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on life satisfaction (β = −0.15, p < 0.001); the model can explain 21.5% of the variance in PSQI, with the direct effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on PSQI (β = 0.346, p < 0.001) being greater than the direct effect of the feeling of money scarcity on PSQI (β = 0.232, p < 0.001); the model can explain 29.9% of the variance in the depression dimension, with the largest direct effect being life satisfaction on depression (β = −0.372, p < 0.001) and the smallest being the feeling of sleep time scarcity on depression (β = −0.043, p < 0.01); the model can explain 40.2% of the variance in the anxiety dimension, with the largest direct effect being PSQI on anxiety (β = 0.524, p < 0.001) and the smallest being the feeling of money scarcity on anxiety (β = 0.068, p < 0.001); the model can explain 22.2% of the variance in the self-denial dimension, with the largest direct effect being life satisfaction on self-denial (β = −0.431, p < 0.001) and the smallest being PSQI on self-denial (β = −0.075, p < 0.001).

images

Testing for multiple mediation effects

As shown in Table 5, the total effect of the feeling of money scarcity on depression is 0.308, with an indirect effect of 0.241, accounting for 78.2% of the total effect. Specifically, the indirect effect of the feeling of money scarcity on depression through PSQI is 0.054 (95% CI = [0.041, 0.071]); through life satisfaction, it is 0.186 (95% CI = [0.161, 0.210]), and the difference between the two is significant. This indicates that in this model, the effect of the feeling of money scarcity on depression is mainly achieved through the mediating effect of life satisfaction. The total effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on depression is 0.094, with an indirect effect of 0.137 and a direct effect of −0.043. Specifically, the indirect effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on depression through PSQI is 0.081 (95% CI = [0.062, 0.103]); through life satisfaction, it is 0.056 (95% CI = [0.044-0.069]), and the difference between the two is not significant. This indicates that in the effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on depression, the mediating effects of PSQI and life satisfaction are essentially consistent.

images

The total effect of the feeling of money scarcity on anxiety is 0.258, with an indirect effect of 0.190, accounting for 73.6% of the total effect. Specifically, the indirect effect of the feeling of money scarcity on anxiety through PSQI is 0.122 (95% CI = [0.100, 0.142]); through life satisfaction, it is 0.068 (95% CI = [0.047, 0.090]), and the difference between the two is significant. This indicates that in this model, the effect of the feeling of money scarcity on anxiety is mainly achieved through the mediating effect of PSQI. The total effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on anxiety is 0.212, with an indirect effect of 0.202 and a non-significant direct effect. Specifically, the indirect effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on anxiety through PSQI is 0.182 (95% CI = [0.154, 0.209]); through life satisfaction, it is 0.020 (95% CI = [0.013, 0.029]), and the difference between the two is significant. This indicates that in this model, the effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on anxiety is mainly achieved through the mediating effect of PSQI.

The total effect of the feeling of money scarcity on self-denial is 0.233, with a direct effect of 0. Specifically, the indirect effect of the feeling of money scarcity on self-denial through PSQI is 0.017 (95% CI = [0.008, 0.030]); through life satisfaction, it is 0.216 (95% CI = [0.191, 0.242]), and the difference between the two is significant. This indicates that in this model, the effect of the feeling of money scarcity on self-denial is mainly achieved through the mediating effect of life satisfaction. The total effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on self-denial is 0.092, with an indirect effect of 0.091 and a non-significant direct effect. Specifically, the indirect effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on self-denial through PSQI is 0.026 (95% CI = [0.011, 0.045]); through life satisfaction, it is 0.065 (95% CI = [0.052, 0.078]), and the difference between the two is significant. This indicates that in this model, the effect of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on self-denial is mainly achieved through the mediating effect of life satisfaction.

The theoretical model diagram after testing is shown in Fig. 2.

images

Figure 2: Model diagram of multiple mediating effects. Notes: Numbers on paths indicate standardized regression coefficients; arrows indicate affective relationships and double arrows indicate correlations; solid lines indicate significant paths, while dashed lines indicate non-significant paths. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

Chinese people have a weak sense of money scarcity but a strong sense of sleep time scarcity; sleep quality is generally good, life satisfaction is average, and mental health is good

First of all, people generally feel that they do not have too much money, and there is a slight sense of money scarcity. Although China will have eliminated absolute poverty by 2021, the shadow of poverty may not have dissipated. Its most common manifestation today is a sense of scarcity. According to the theory of relative deprivation, increasing socioeconomic inequality may cause more people to feel disadvantaged relative to the wealthier segments of society, and people tend to compare their situation with others or desired standards, creating a sense of psychological disadvantage and resource scarcity [78]. In addition, as the cost of living rises, especially in key areas such as education, health care, and housing, a portion of the middle-income level may also feel financial stress and a sense of monetary scarcity. The problem of the sense of money scarcity reflected in our findings is not acute due to the fact that China’s economy has shown great resilience and solidity in the face of the complex international and domestic situation. Especially after the epidemic, China’s economy has shown a good trend of rapid recovery, which, to a certain extent, is conducive to guaranteeing people’s basic living needs, stabilizing the job market, and maintaining the overall stability of society.

Secondly, people have a strong sense of scarcity of sleep time. In a competitive social environment, individuals compete and struggle for higher social status and economic benefits, leading to increasing pressure in work and life and the phenomenon of “involution”. In the face of intense work tasks and deadlines, people often need to work overtime, and this high-intensity work pressure not only leads to anxiety and other psychological problems but also disrupts the normal routine of work and rest, making it difficult for people to get enough sleep.

Third, the average score of PSQI is low, indicating that people have good sleep quality. It is well known that unhealthy lifestyles and excessive use of electronic devices (especially before bedtime) are two major factors affecting sleep quality [7982]. Better sleep quality may be due to the fact that people have begun to focus on cultivating good sleep habits in their daily lives, such as setting regular work and rest schedules and avoiding excessive use of electronic devices; they may also have optimized their sleep environment, such as creating a quiet and comfortable bedroom environment to promote better sleep. This is a positive result that can reflect people’s attention to their own sleep health and initiative to improve it. The next study should pay more attention to groups with average to low sleep quality and special groups and continue to explore the factors affecting their sleep quality, causes of sleep problems, and coping strategies.

Fourth, people’s life satisfaction is average. Life satisfaction refers to people’s overall comprehensive evaluation of their own lives [83] and is influenced by a variety of factors such as personal values, expectations, experiences, and environments. At the societal level, promoting economic growth, protecting and promoting social trust, and reducing income inequality are all conducive to improving life satisfaction [84,85]; at the individual level, positive social behaviors and social connections [86], personal life events, and social support [43] are all conducive to improving life satisfaction. Thus, improvements in life satisfaction require both individual and social facilitation. Implementing effective interventions at the individual level [87], the group level and the societal level is and is necessary [88,89]. Fifth, the mental health of the population is good. Health is a state of “complete physical, mental, and social well-being, not merely the absence of disease or infirmity” [90]. Further, positive health predicts increased longevity (corrected for quality of life), lower health costs, better mental health during aging, and better prognosis for disease coming to the forefront [91]. Our findings reflect the fact that most people are able to effectively maintain a positive state of mind. This state of mental health may be related to the improvement of the social environment, the promotion of mental health education, and the importance people place on mental health.

The stronger the feeling of money scarcity, the higher the levels of depression and anxiety; the stronger the feeling of sleep time scarcity, the higher the level of depression

The feeling of money scarcity or sleep time scarcity is associated with different dimensions of mental health issues, which is consistent with previous research [34,35,74,75]. Money and time represent different health resources that are not interchangeable, but having both amplifies the effects on health [7]. Scarcity is a pervasive real-life psychological phenomenon in which individuals feel a scarcity of money or time regardless of their economic status. The feeling of scarcity affects the way people allocate their attention, causing them to pay more attention to the problems they face in the present but, at the same time, neglecting the problems of the future [11]. This behavioral phenomenon is known as the “pipe dream effect”, which causes the vicious circle of “scarcity-irrational decision-making-scarcity”, making it more difficult for individuals to get rid of the scarcity state, and thus falling into a downward spiral in a dilemma. The individual will be trapped in a downward spiral. Meanwhile, long-term exposure to resource scarcity affects individuals’ willingness to delay gratification, orientation toward others, self-esteem, and materialism, and even if resources become abundant, the negative consequences of resource scarcity will have a long-term impact on individuals [6,92,93]. In addition, cognitive consistency theory suggests that individuals tend to maintain consistency by adjusting their perceptions, even if this means changing their attitudes, beliefs, or behaviors. Thus, individuals’ feelings, attitudes, and thoughts play a decisive role in their behavior [94], i.e., once an individual subjectively identifies himself/herself as a “scarce” resource, he/she will adjust his/her attitudes, beliefs, or values to be consistent with this “scarce” cognitive element, in any state. “This will seriously affect and limit the survival and development of the individual. Therefore, understanding and managing the sense of scarcity is crucial to improving an individual’s mental health and quality of life, for example, by developing rational time management and financial planning and by seeking external support and help to mitigate the negative impact of scarcity on mental health.

The feeling of scarcity can lower sleep quality or life satisfaction, leading to depression, anxiety, and self-denial, thereby affecting mental health

Firstly, the effect of the feeling of money scarcity on depression is mainly achieved through the mediating effect of life satisfaction, while in the pathway of the feeling of sleep time scarcity’s effect on depression, the mediating effects of PSQI and life satisfaction are largely consistent. In the perspective of cultural theory, money is not just a neutral medium for transactions; it is also full of symbolic meanings and an important indicator of individual or family social status. In many cultures, the amount of wealth is often linked to personal success and social status [95], where individuals with greater wealth may be seen as more influential and respected. Therefore, money becomes a means to display and reinforce social status. Additionally, how individuals use and spend money reflects their values, tastes, and lifestyles [96,97]. Thus, compared to the feeling of sleep time scarcity, the feeling of money scarcity may touch upon an individual’s sense of survival security, control over life, and social identity, thereby having a more direct and stronger impact on life satisfaction.

Furthermore, the effects of the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity on anxiety are mainly achieved through the mediating effect of PSQI. Individuals under greater economic pressure are more likely to experience sleep problems such as insomnia and decreased sleep quality, which can lead to anxiety. Moreover, as the pace of society accelerates, people face increasing life stress, resulting in reduced sleep time. Sleep time scarcity can cause individuals to feel fatigued and have reduced concentration during the day, leading to anxiety. This conclusion helps us better understand the mechanisms behind anxiety and provides insights for its prevention and alleviation. In daily life, it is important to pay attention to personal money and time management, ensure an adequate amount of sleep, and maintain psychological well-being.

Lastly, the effects of the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity on self-denial are mainly achieved through the mediating effect of life satisfaction. The feeling of money scarcity not only leads individuals to worry about their quality of life but also can create feelings of inferiority in interpersonal interactions, leading to social barriers that lower life satisfaction. Additionally, the feeling of money scarcity can make individuals overly focused on material wealth, neglecting other aspects of life pursuits, which can result in decreased life satisfaction. This decline in life satisfaction may cause individuals to doubt their abilities and worth, leading to self-denial. On the other hand, the feeling of sleep time scarcity can result in physical and mental exhaustion, as well as emotional fluctuations, impacting an individual’s cognitive and emotional regulation abilities. This state of fatigue and instability can lead individuals to question their abilities and worth, affecting their social skills and relationships, ultimately reducing life satisfaction and triggering self-denial.

Limitations and Prospects

Although the relationship between the feeling of scarcity and psychological well-being has been widely confirmed, this study represents a novel attempt to explore the mechanisms underlying this relationship. On one hand, introducing life satisfaction as a mediating variable is an innovative approach; on the other hand, integrating the scarcity perspective into the study of sleep behavior in the health behavior domain, in line with current realities, validates the impact mechanism of the feeling of sleep time scarcity on psychological well-being. This not only enriches the related research on time scarcity but also broadens the scope of sleep research, facilitating innovative sleep intervention measures and approaches. However, this study also has some limitations: firstly, the data used in the study are all based on self-reporting by participants, which may introduce response bias affecting the accuracy of the results; secondly, due to the cross-sectional nature of the data used in the study, it is not possible to conclusively infer causality between the feeling of scarcity and psychological well-being; thirdly, this study is based on measuring the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity within the social and cultural context of China, and the results may not necessarily be generalizable to other cultural backgrounds.

In conclusion, there exists a complex relationship between sleep quality, life satisfaction, and psychological well-being. The results of this study provide only a partial understanding of the relationship between the three. In future research, it is suggested to: firstly, use experimental methods to collect objective indicators of variables to enhance result accuracy; secondly, measure the samples at multiple time points to obtain longitudinal data for inferring causal relationships between variables; thirdly, consider validating results in different cultural contexts or introducing other relevant variables or mediating variables to construct models for in-depth exploration of the impact mechanisms of different types of scarcity on individual physical and mental health; fourthly, introduce demographic characteristics such as gender, age, education level, household income, and socioeconomic status as moderating variables to examine differences in the types of scarcity among different groups.

Conclusion

This study, through empirical research, has found that the stronger the feeling of money scarcity, the higher the levels of depression and anxiety, the stronger the feeling of sleep time scarcity, the higher the level of depression. The feeling of money scarcity or the feeling of sleep time scarcity will both lower sleep quality or life satisfaction, leading to depression, anxiety, and self-denial, impacting mental health. We have not only confirmed the correlation between the feeling of money scarcity and the feeling of sleep time scarcity with different dimensions of mental health but also revealed the potential impact pathways of scarcity on individual mental health dimensions. This provides insight and guidance for promoting healthy lifestyles and improving mental health. Therefore, under objectively feasible conditions, it is important to plan finances and time reasonably, reduce the feeling of scarcity mentality, cultivate an optimistic and positive attitude towards life, be content, live in the present, and avoid falling into the “scarcity trap.” Furthermore, improving sleep quality and life satisfaction can reduce the adverse effects of the feeling of scarcity on mental health.

Acknowledgement: We thank the associate editor and the reviewers for their useful feedback that improved this paper.

Funding Statement: This study was funded by the Key Projects of Philosophy and Social Sciences Research, Ministry of Education (Grant Number 21JZD038).

Author Contributions: The authors confirm contribution to the paper as follows: study conception and design: Na Liu, Yan Zhang; data collection: Junxiu Wang; analysis and interpretation of results: Na Liu, Yan Zhang; draft manuscript preparation: Na Liu. All authors reviewed the results and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Availability of Data and Materials: The data are now confidential and for internal use only, but they will be shared with the public soon. To know more information about the data, please refer to the Chinese Social Survey (http://css.cssn.cn) conducted by the same institution. Both surveys used the same sampling framework and some of their questions were the same.

Ethics Approval: The study was approved by the Academic Committee of the Institute of Sociology, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences (no IRB number). All participants signed the informed consent in this study.

Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest to report regarding the present study.

References

1. 2022 China Labor Statistics Yearbook. Average weekly working hours of urban employed persons surveyed. 2022. Available from: https://www.zgtjnj.org/navipage-n3023042404000155.html. [Accessed 2024]. [Google Scholar]

2. Labor Law of the People’s Republic of China. 1995. Available from: https://baike.so.com/doc/30506719-32302201.html. [Accessed 2024]. [Google Scholar]

3. Barnes CM, Wagner DT, Ghumman S. Borrowing from sleep to pay work and family: expanding time-based conflict to the broader nonwork domain. Pers Psychol. 2012;65(4):789–819. doi:10.1111/peps.2012.65.issue-4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

4. Fernbach PM, Kan C, Lynch JG. Squeezed: coping with constraint through efficiency and prioritization. J Consum Res. 2015;41(5):1204–27. doi:10.1086/679118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

5. Griskevicius V, Ackerman JM, Cantú SM, Delton AW, Robertson TE, Simpson JA, et al. When the economy falters, do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on childhood environments. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(2):197–205. doi:10.1177/0956797612451471. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

6. Hamilton R, Thompson D, Bone S, Chaplin LN, Griskevicius V, Goldsmith K, et al. The effects of scarcity on consumer decision journeys. J Acad Market Sci. 2019;47(3):532–50. doi:10.1007/s11747-018-0604-7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

7. Venn D, Strazdins L. Your money or your time? How both types of scarcity matter to physical activity and healthy eating. Soc Sci Med. 2017;172:98–106. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.10.023. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

8. Mullainathan S, Shafir E. Scarcity: why having too little means so much. New York: Times Books; 2013. [Google Scholar]

9. Kahneman D. Attention and effort. Citeseer: Prentice Hall; 1973. [Google Scholar]

10. Huijsmans I, Ma I, Micheli L, Civai C, Stallen M, Sanfey AG. A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2019;116(24):11699–704. doi:10.1073/pnas.1818572116. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

11. Shah AK, Mullainathan S, Shafir E. Some consequences of having too little. Science. 2012;338(6107):682–5. doi:10.1126/science.1222426. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

12. Cannon C, Goldsmith K, Roux C. A self-regulatory model of resource scarcity. J Consum Psychol. 2019;29(1):104–27. doi:10.1002/jcpy.2019.29.issue-1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

13. Gee GC, Hing A, Mohammed S, Tabor DC, Williams DR. Racism and the life course: taking time seriously. Am J Public Health. 2019;109(S1):S43–7. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2018.304766. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

14. Whillans AV, Dunn EW, Smeets P, Bekkers R, Norton MI. Buying time promotes happiness. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017;114(32):8523–7. doi:10.1073/pnas.1706541114. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

15. Bubu OM, Brannick M, Mortimer J, Umasabor-Bubu O, Sebastião YV, Wen Y, et al. Sleep, cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep. 2017;40(1):zsw032. [Google Scholar]

16. McSorley VE, Bin YS, Lauderdale DS. Associations of sleep characteristics with cognitive function and decline among older adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2019;188(6):1066–75. doi:10.1093/aje/kwz037. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

17. Sewell KR, Erickson KI, Rainey-Smith SR, Peiffer JJ, Sohrabi HR, Brown BM. Relationships between physical activity, sleep and cognitive function: a narrative review. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2021;130:369–78. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.003. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

18. Irwin MR, Opp MR. Sleep health: reciprocal regulation of sleep and innate immunity. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;42(1):129–55. doi:10.1038/npp.2016.148. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

19. Prather AA, Janicki-Deverts D, Hall MH, Cohen S. Behaviorally assessed sleep and susceptibility to the common cold. Sleep. 2015;38(9):1353–9. doi:10.5665/sleep.4968. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

20. Besedovsky L, Lange T, Haack M. The sleep-immune crosstalk in health and disease. Physiol Rev. 2019;99(3):1325–80. doi:10.1152/physrev.00010.2018. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

21. Tobaldini E, Fiorelli EM, Solbiati M, Costantino G, Nobili L, Montano N. Short sleep duration and cardiometabolic risk: from pathophysiology to clinical evidence. Nat Rev Cardiol. 2019;16(4):213–24. doi:10.1038/s41569-018-0109-6. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

22. Sejbuk M, Mirończuk-Chodakowska I, Witkowska AM. Sleep quality: a narrative review on nutrition, stimulants, and physical activity as important factors. Nutrients. 2022;14(9):1912. doi:10.3390/nu14091912. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

23. Pepin JL, Borel AL, Tamisier R, Baguet JP, Levy P, Dauvilliers Y. Hypertension and sleep: overview of a tight relationship. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18(6):509–19. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2014.03.003. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

24. Gunn HE, Eberhardt KR. Family dynamics in sleep health and hypertension. Curr Hypertens Rep. 2019;21(5):39. doi:10.1007/s11906-019-0944-9. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

25. Grandner MA, Jackson N, Gerstner JR, Knutson KL. Sleep symptoms associated with intake of specific dietary nutrients. J Sleep Res. 2014;23(1):22–34. doi:10.1111/jsr.2014.23.issue-1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

26. Möller-Levet CS, Archer SN, Bucca G, Laing EE, Slak A, Kabiljo R, et al. Effects of insufficient sleep on circadian rhythmicity and expression amplitude of the human blood transcriptome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(12):E1132–41. [Google Scholar]

27. Broussard JL, Ehrmann DA, Van Cauter E, Tasali E, Brady MJ. Impaired insulin signaling in human adipocytes after experimental sleep restriction: a randomized, crossover study. Ann Intern Med. 2012;157(8):549–57. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-157-8-201210160-00005. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

28. Wang L, He S, Yan N, Pan R, Niu Y, Li J. Mediating role of depressive symptoms on the relationship between sleep duration and cognitive function. Sci Rep. 2023;13(1):4067. doi:10.1038/s41598-023-31357-6. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

29. Marino C, Andrade B, Montplaisir J, Petit D, Touchette E, Paradis H, et al. Testing bidirectional, longitudinal associations between disturbed sleep and depressive symptoms in children and adolescents using cross-lagged models. Jama Netw Open. 2022;5(8):e2227119. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.27119. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

30. Kalenkoski CM, Hamrick KS. How does time poverty affect behavior? A look at eating and physical activity. Appl Econ Perspect P. 2013;35(1):89–105. doi:10.1093/aepp/pps034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

31. Strazdins L, Griffin AL, Broom DH, Banwell C, Korda R, Dixon J, et al. Time scarcity: another health inequality? Environ Plann A. 2011;43(3):545–59. doi:10.1068/a4360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

32. Humphreys BR, Ruseski JE. An economic analysis of participation and time spent in physical activity. The B.E. J Econ Anal Policy. 2011;11(1):1–38. [Google Scholar]

33. Cleland V, Ball K, Crawford D. Socioeconomic position and physical activity among women in Melbourne, Australia: does the use of different socioeconomic indicators matter? Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(10):1578–83. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.01.031. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

34. Roxburgh S. ‘There just aren’t enough hours in the day’: the mental health consequences of time pressure. J Health Soc Behav. 2004;45(2):115–31. doi:10.1177/002214650404500201. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

35. Höge T. When work strain transcends psychological boundaries: an inquiry into the relationship between time pressure, irritation, work-family conflict and psychosomatic complaints. Stress Health. 2009;25(1):41–51. doi:10.1002/smi.v25:1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

36. Nyarko SH, Luo L, Schlundt DG, Xiao Q. Individual and neighborhood socioeconomic status and long-term individual trajectories of sleep duration among black and white adults: the Southern Community Cohort Study. Sleep. 2023;46(1):zsac225. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsac225. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

37. Etindele Sosso FA, Kreidlmayer M, Pearson D, Bendaoud I. Towards a socioeconomic model of sleep health among the canadian population: a systematic review of the relationship between age, income, employment, education, social class, socioeconomic status and sleep disparities. Eur J Invest Health. 2022;12(8):1143–67. [Google Scholar]

38. Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychol Bull. 1984;95(3):542–75. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

39. Diener E, Biswas-Diener R. Will money increase subjective well-being? Soc Indic Res. 2002;57(2):119–69. doi:10.1023/A:1014411319119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

40. Diener E, Oishi S, Lucas RE. Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54(1):403–25. doi:10.1146/psych.2003.54.issue-1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

41. Cheung F, Lucas RE. When does money matter most? Examining the association between income and life satisfaction over the life course. Psychol Aging. 2015;30(1):120–35. doi:10.1037/a0038682. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

42. Stanley S, Balakrishnan S. Family Caregiving in Schizophrenia: do stress, social support and resilience influence life satisfaction?–A quantitative study from India. Soc Work Ment Health. 2023;21(1):67–85. doi:10.1080/15332985.2022.2070051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

43. Khodabakhsh S. Factors affecting life satisfaction of older adults in Asia: a systematic review. J Happiness Stud. 2022;23(3):1289–304. doi:10.1007/s10902-021-00433-x. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

44. Bakkeli NZ. Health, work, and contributing factors on life satisfaction: a study in Norway before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. SSM Popul Health. 2021;14:100804. doi:10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100804. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

45. Liu LK. The healthier older people are, the more satisfied they are with their lives?—Test from 2014 CLHLS data of age, urban and rural and gender. J Shijiazhuang Railway Univ (Soc Sci Ed). 2021;15(2):45–51 (In Chinese). [Google Scholar]

46. Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L. Advances in subjective well-being research. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(4):253–60. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

47. Huppert FA, So TTC. Flourishing across Europe: application of a new conceptual framework for defining well-being. Soc Indic Res. 2013;110(3):837–61. doi:10.1007/s11205-011-9966-7. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

48. Graham C, Nikolova M. Does access to information technology make people happier? Insights from well-being surveys from around the world. J Soc Econ. 2013;44:126–39. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2013.02.025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

49. Lucas RE, Clark AE, Georgellis Y, Diener E. Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: reactions to changes in marital status. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84(3):527–39. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.527. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

50. Kang W. Illegal drug use is associated with poorer life satisfaction and self-rated health (SRH) in young people. Front Psychiatry. 2023;14:955626. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2023.955626. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

51. Kim BJ, Chen L, Xu L, Lee Y. Self-rated health and subjective economic status in life satisfaction among older chinese immigrants: a cross-sectional study. Healthc Basel. 2021;9(3):342. [Google Scholar]

52. Rajabi Gilan N, Khezeli M, Zardoshtian S. The effect of self-rated health, subjective socioeconomic status, social capital, and physical activity on life satisfaction: a cross-sectional study in urban western Iran. BMC Public Health. 2021;21(1):233. doi:10.1186/s12889-021-10261-6. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

53. Wang X, Wang P, Wang P, Cao M, Xu X. Relationships among mental health, social capital and life satisfaction in rural senior older adults: a structural equation model. BMC Geriatr. 2022;22(1):73. doi:10.1186/s12877-022-02761-w. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

54. Lucas JJ, Moore KA. Psychological flexibility: positive implications for mental health and life satisfaction. Health Promot Int. 2020;35(2):312–20. doi:10.1093/heapro/daz036. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

55. Divine A, Blanchard C, Benoit C, Downs DS, Rhodes RE. The influence of sleep and movement on mental health and life satisfaction during the transition to parenthood. Sleep Health. 2022;8(5):475–83. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2022.06.013. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

56. Lee B. A serial mediation model of gratitude on life satisfaction in people with multiple sclerosis: the intermediary role of perceived stress and mental health symptoms. Mult Scler Relat Dis. 2022;58:103421. doi:10.1016/j.msard.2021.103421. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

57. Dong Y, Xu L, Wu S, Qin W, Hu F, Li M, et al. The mediating effect of perceived social support on mental health and life satisfaction among residents: a cross-sectional analysis of 8500 subjects in Taian City, China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(22):14756. doi:10.3390/ijerph192214756. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

58. Nakamura JS, Delaney SW, Diener E, VanderWeele TJ, Kim ES. Are all domains of life satisfaction equal? Differential associations with health and well-being in older adults. Qual Life Res. 2022;31(4):1043–56. doi:10.1007/s11136-021-02977-0. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

59. Kim ES, Delaney SW, Tay L, Chen Y, Diener E, Vanderweele TJ. Life satisfaction and subsequent physical, behavioral, and psychosocial health in older adults. Milbank Q. 2021;99(1):209–39. doi:10.1111/milq.v99.1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

60. Fergusson DM, McLeod GFH, Horwood LJ, Swain NR, Chapple S, Poulton R. Life satisfaction and mental health problems (18 to 35 years). Psychol Med. 2015;45(11):2427–36. doi:10.1017/S0033291715000422. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

61. Urbanova LB, Holubcikova J, Madarasova Geckova A, Reijneveld SA, van Dijk JP. Does life satisfaction mediate the association between socioeconomic status and excessive internet use? IJERPH. 2019;16(20):3914. doi:10.3390/ijerph16203914. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

62. Lu R, Wang Z, Lin X, Guo L. How do family role overload and work interferance with family affect the life satisfaction and sleep sufficiency of construction professionals? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(17):3094. doi:10.3390/ijerph16173094. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

63. Pan L, Li L, Peng H, Fan L, Liao J, Wang M, et al. Association of depressive symptoms with marital status among the middle-aged and elderly in Rural China-serial mediating effects of sleep time, pain and life satisfaction. J Affect Disord. 2022;303:52–7. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2022.01.111. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

64. Wang M, Pan L, Peng H, Song B, Zeng Y, Qian M, et al. Chinese visceral adiposity index and depressive symptoms in middle-aged and elderly Chinese: dose-response correlation and the effect mediated by sleep time and life satisfaction. Depress Anxiety. 2023;2023:1–10. [Google Scholar]

65. Wang Y, Peng J. Work-family conflict and depression in chinese professional women: the mediating roles of job satisfaction and life satisfaction. Int J Ment Health Ad. 2017;15(2):394–406. doi:10.1007/s11469-017-9736-0. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

66. Almeida D, Monteiro D, Rodrigues F. Satisfaction with life: mediating role in the relationship between depressive symptoms and coping mechanisms. Healthcare. 2021;9(7):787. doi:10.3390/healthcare9070787. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

67. Jachimowicz JM, Frey EL, Matz SC, Jeronimus BF, Galinsky AD. The sharp spikes of poverty: financial scarcity is related to higher levels of distress intensity in daily life. Soc Psychol Pers Sci. 2022;13(8):1187–98. doi:10.1177/19485506211060115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

68. Haushofer J, Fehr E. On the psychology of poverty. Science. 2014;344(6186):862–7. doi:10.1126/science.1232491. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

69. Kim JH, Park EC. Impact of socioeconomic status and subjective social class on overall and health-related quality of life. BMC Public Health. 2015;15(1):783. doi:10.1186/s12889-015-2014-9. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

70. Muscatell KA. Socioeconomic influences on brain function: implications for health. Ann Ny Acad Sci. 2018;1428(1):14–32. doi:10.1111/nyas.2018.1428.issue-1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

71. Chan CMH, Wong JE, Yeap LLL, Wee LH, Jamil NA, Swarna Nantha Y. Workplace bullying and psychological distress of employees across socioeconomic strata: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(S4):608. doi:10.1186/s12889-019-6859-1. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

72. Hutton M. Consuming stress: exploring hidden dimensions of consumption-related strain at the intersection of gender and poverty. J Market Manag. 2015;31(15–16):1695–717. [Google Scholar]

73. Greitemeyer T, Sagioglou C. Subjective socioeconomic status causes aggression: a test of the theory of social deprivation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2016;111(2):178–94. doi:10.1037/pspi0000058. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

74. Kasser T, Sheldon KM. Time affluence as a path toward personal happiness and ethical business practice: empirical evidence from four studies. J Bus Ethics. 2009;84(S2):243–55. doi:10.1007/s10551-008-9696-1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

75. Li H, Boey KW. Assessing psychological well-being of college students: psychometric properties of GHQ-20. Psychol Dev Educ. 2002;1:75–9. [Google Scholar]

76. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiat Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

77. Diener ED, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess. 1985;49(1):71–5. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

78. Sharma E, Alter AL. Financial deprivation prompts consumers to seek scarce goods. J Consum Res. 2012;39(3):545–60. doi:10.1086/664038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

79. Fuentes-Senise C, García-Corpas JP. Prevalence of poor sleep quality and associated lifestyle habits: a cross-sectional study in community pharmacies. Ars Pharm. 2022;64(1):5–18. doi:10.30827/ars.v64i1.26223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

80. Shochat T. Impact of lifestyle and technology developments on sleep. Nat Sci Sleep. 2012;4:19–31. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]

81. Exelmans L, van den Bulck J. Bedtime mobile phone use and sleep in adults. Soc Sci Med. 2016;148:93–101. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.11.037. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

82. Alonzo R, Hussain J, Stranges S, Anderson KK. Interplay between social media use, sleep quality, and mental health in youth: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2021;56:101414. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2020.101414. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

83. Veenhoven R. Happiness: also known as “life satisfaction” and “subjective well-being”. In: Handbook of social indicators and quality of life research. Netherlands: Springer; 2011. p. 63–77. [Google Scholar]

84. Kuo CT, Kawachi I. Relative deprivation and human flourishing: how do upward income comparisons affect health, happiness and life satisfaction? J Epidemiol Commun H. 2023;77(10):656–62. doi:10.1136/jech-2023-220582. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

85. Mikucka M, Sarracino F, Dubrow JK. When does economic growth improve life satisfaction? Multilevel analysis of the roles of social trust and income inequality in 46 countries, 1981–2012. World Dev. 2017;93:447–59. doi:10.1016/j.worlddev.2017.01.002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

86. Regan A, Radošić N, Lyubomirsky S. Experimental effects of social behavior on well-being. Trends Cogn Sci. 2022;26(11):987–98. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2022.08.006. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

87. Wiese CW, Kuykendall L, Tay L. Get active? A meta-analysis of leisure-time physical activity and subjective well-being. J Posit Psychol. 2018;13(1):57–66. doi:10.1080/17439760.2017.1374436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

88. Ko H, Jung S. Association of social frailty with physical health, cognitive function, psychological health, and life satisfaction in community-dwelling older Koreans. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(2):818. doi:10.3390/ijerph18020818. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

89. Bolier L, Haverman M, Westerhof GJ, Riper H, Smit F, Bohlmeijer E. Positive psychology interventions: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):119. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-13-119. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

90. WHO Interim Commission. Official Records of the World Health Organization No. 2: Summary Report on Proceedings, Minutes and Final Acts of the International Health Conference held in New York from 19 June to 22 July 1946. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1948. Available from: https://iris.who.int/bitstream/handle/10665/85573/Official_record2_eng.pdf;sequence=1. [Accessed 2021]. [Google Scholar]

91. Seligman MEP. Positive health. Appl Psychol. 2008;57(s1):3–18. doi:10.1111/apps.2008.57.issue-s1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

92. Mittal C, Laran J, Griskevicius V. How early-life resource scarcity influences self-confidence and task completion judgments. J Assoc Consum Res. 2020;5(4):404–14, 2378-1815. [Google Scholar]

93. Amir D, Jordan MR, Rand DG. An uncertainty management perspective on long-run impacts of adversity: the influence of childhood socioeconomic status on risk, time, and social preferences. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2018;79:217–26. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2018.07.014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

94. Festinger L. Cognitive dissonance. Sci Am. 1962;207(4):93–106. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican1062-93. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [CrossRef]

95. Wingen T, Englich B, Estal-Muñoz V, Mareva S, Kassianos AP. Exploring the relationship between social class and quality of life: the mediating role of power and status. Appl Res Qual Life. 2021;16(5):1983–98. doi:10.1007/s11482-020-09853-y. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

96. Bourdieu P. Distinction a social critique of the judgement of taste. In: Inequality. Abingdon: Routledge; 2018. p. 287–318. [Google Scholar]

97. Veblen T. The theory of the leisure class. Routledge: Elibron Classics; 2017. p. 1899. [Google Scholar]


Cite This Article

APA Style
Liu, N., Zhang, Y., Wang, J. (2024). Scarcity and mental health—multiple mediators of sleep quality and life satisfaction. International Journal of Mental Health Promotion, 26(6), 449-462. https://doi.org/10.32604/ijmhp.2024.049334
Vancouver Style
Liu N, Zhang Y, Wang J. Scarcity and mental health—multiple mediators of sleep quality and life satisfaction. Int J Ment Health Promot. 2024;26(6):449-462 https://doi.org/10.32604/ijmhp.2024.049334
IEEE Style
N. Liu, Y. Zhang, and J. Wang, “Scarcity and Mental Health—Multiple Mediators of Sleep Quality and Life Satisfaction,” Int. J. Ment. Health Promot., vol. 26, no. 6, pp. 449-462, 2024. https://doi.org/10.32604/ijmhp.2024.049334


cc Copyright © 2024 The Author(s). Published by Tech Science Press.
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License , which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
  • 762

    View

  • 310

    Download

  • 0

    Like

Share Link